42 The Adjustment Puts Bones out of Place?
Printer Friendly Email a Friend PDF RSS Feed

Dynamic Chiropractic – December 17, 2007, Vol. 25, Issue 26

The Adjustment Puts Bones out of Place?

By David Seaman, DC, MS, DABCN

It's illogical to make definitive statements about the mechanical and neurobiological effects of spinal adjustments, as we simply do not have adequate evidence to support such a position. The term subluxation typically is characterized as a spinal bone that has moved out of position relative to the adjacent vertebrae.

Patients often will say they feel "out" and need an adjustment to get realigned.

Most patients and docs feel that a successful adjustment is one that is associated with the cracking sound of spinal joint cavitation. While there is no evidence to suggest that an adjustment must be accompanied by a cavitation, we all tend to like it better and feel more confident that a meaningful event occurred when we hear the cavitation.

The cracking sound often is followed with patient/doctor comments about how the vertebra "popped" or "moved" back into place or alignment, or the joint was reset. In short, numerous and various terms are used to describe how misaligned vertebrae are "adjusted back into place."

image - Copyright – Stock Photo / Register Mark If, in fact, the adjustment reset the vertebral position, we should be able to see such a change on X-ray or MRI. Unfortunately, very few attempts have been made to demonstrate this outcome. However, for those who believe that "bones pop back into place," there is strong contrary evidence. In 2003, Dr. Greg Cramer and colleagues published the results of a study that suggested a very different outcome occurs with lumbar spine adjusting.1

Lumbar MRIs were first taken of subjects in a side-posture position to visualize both zygapophyseal joints (Z-joints). Then subjects were given a side-posture adjustment and a second MRI was taken. Figure 1 illustrates the pre- and post-adjustment images in the Z-joints.

Clearly, we can see that the left lumbar Z-joint gapped as a consequence of an adjustive thrust. We are told that the right Z-joint was simultaneously moved into a closed packed position.1 Each change was transient in nature. This biomechanical outcome of spinal adjusting supports Sandoz's hypothesis published back in 1976.2

Based on this image, we clearly can see that adjustments don't put bones back into place. If anything, just the opposite occurs in the short term. In addition to moving vertebrae temporarily out of position, we also know that adjustments improve movement asymmetries3-7 and reduce spinal muscle excitability,8,9 and it's well-established that adjustments reduce pain for many patients, which is why it's recommended in the various guidelines for treating low back pain.

Such outcomes of spinal adjusting should lead us to consider the possible nature of subluxation in a different light than the traditional view that subluxation is a bone out of place, which causes nerve interference. In fact, the evidence suggests the opposite.

References

  1. Cramer GD, Gregerson DM, Knudsen JT, et al. The effects of side-posture positioning and spinal adjusting on the lumbar Z joints: a randomized controlled trial with sixty-four subjects. Spine, 2002;27(22):2459-66.
  2. Sandoz R. Some physical mechanisms and effects of spinal adjustments. Ann Swiss Chiropr Assoc, 1976;6:91-141.
  3. Carrick FR. Cervical radiculopathy: the diagnosis and treatment of pathomechanics in the cervical spine. J Manipulative Physiol Ther, 1983;6:129-37.
  4. Nansel DD, Cremeta E, Carlson J, Szlazak M. Time course considerations for the effects of unilateral lower cervical adjustments with respect to the amelioration of cervical lateral-flexion passive end-range asymmetry. J Manipulative Physiol Ther, 1990;13:297-304.
  5. Nansel D, Jansen R, Cremata E, et al. Effects of cervical adjustments on lateral-flexion passive end-range asymmetry and on blood pressure, heart rate and plasma catecholamine levels. J Manipulative Physiol Ther, 1991;14:450-6.
  6. Nansel DD, Peneff A, Quitoriano J. Effectiveness of upper versus lower cervical adjustments with respect to the amelioration of passive rotational versus lateral-flexion end-range asymmetries in otherwise asymptomatic subjects. J Manipulative Physiol Ther, 1992;15:99-105.
  7. Nansel DD, Waldorf T, Cooperstein R. Time course effect of cervical spinal adjustments on lumbar paraspinal muscle tone: evidence for facilitation of intersegmental tonic neck reflexes. J Manipulative Physiol Ther, 1993;16(2):91-5.
  8. Lehman GJ, McGill SM. The influence of a chiropractic manipulation on lumbar kinematics and electromyography during simple and complex tasks: a case study. J Manipulative Physiol Ther, 1999;22:576-81.
  9. Lehman GJ, McGill SM. Spinal manipulation causes variable spine kinematic and trunk muscle electromyographic responses. Clin Biomech, 2001;16:293-9.

Click here for more information about David Seaman, DC, MS, DABCN.


To report inappropriate ads, click here.